阅读文献:

Tsunada, J., Liu, A., Gold, J. et al. Erratum: Causal contribution of primate auditory cortex to auditory perceptual decision-making. Nat Neurosci 19, 642 (2016).

文献链接:

Erratum: Causal contribution of primate auditory cortex to auditory perceptual decision-making | Nature Neuroscience ​​​​​​

本文为参加北京脑科学与类脑研究中心CIBR2022年冬季培训班Journal Club的文献阅读报告,感谢CIBR提供的学习机会和平台。

Contents

  • Abstract
  • Introduction
  • 1. Research aim
  • 2. Methods
  • 3. Results
  • 3.1 Behavioral performance
  • 3.2 Recording-site localization
  • 3.3 Neuronal stimulus sensitivity
  • 3.4 Neuronal choice sensitivity
  • 3.5 Microstimulation
  • 4. Discussion
  • 5. Conclusions

Abstract

1) Both ML and AL neural activity was modulated by the frequency content of the stimulus

2) The responses of  AL(anterolateral) rather than ML(middle-lateral)  belt regions of the auditory cortex neurons were systematically modulated by the monkeys’ choices and microstimulation.

3) AL directly and causally contributes sensory evidence to form this auditory decision.


Introduction


1. Research aim

Identify whether ML or AL auditory-driven responses are used as evidence to form decisions about the frequency content of auditory stimuli.

2. Methods

Recorded and manipulated ML and AL spiking activity in monkeys while made a difficult decision about whether a noisy stimulus contained more low- or high-frequency tone bursts (Fig. 1).


3. Results

3.1 Behavioral performance

Psychometric data fit by logistic function Chronometric data fit by DDM function Spearman’s correlation coefficient
Dependent variable Percent of high-frequency choices Response time /

Slope/

sensitivity

monkey T: 0.7 [0.5–0.7]

monkey A: 0.8 [0.7-1.0]

monkey T: 0.6[0.5–0.7]

monkey A: 0.7 [0.6-0.8]

R=0.70, P = 2.2 ×10^(-14)
Choice biases

monkey T: 13 [-5–31]% coherence

monkey A: -22 [-30–7]% coherence

monkey T: 14 [-0.7–27]% coherence

monkey A: -7 [-15–4]% coherence

R=0.96, P = 2.7 × 10^(-56)

1) From the psychometric functions (choices)

①  High accuracy for high- and low-coherence stimuli

→ The monkeys were attentive and followed the rules of the task.

② Steepness of the logistic psychometric (choice)  function: 

monkey T: 0.7 [0.5–0.7] (median [interquartile range] across sessions)

monkey A: 0.8 [0.7-1.0]

→ The monkeys used relevant information from the auditory stimuli to inform their decisions.

③ Choice biases measured as the coherence value corresponding to 50% high-frequency choices from the logistic fits

monkey T: 13 [-5–31]% coherence

monkey A: -22 [-30–7]% coherence

→ The monkeys were also relatively unbiased, making roughly equal numbers of low- and high-frequency choices.

2) From the psychometric (choice) and chronometric (RT) functions

① Fit the two functions to drift-diffusion models (DDM) of decision-making with solid red curves.

② Non-decision time (NDT) was fit as a separate free parameter in the DDM for each of the two choices and included stimulus-encoding and motor-preparation times with dashed gray lines in chronometric functions.

③ Decision epoch (Decision time,DT) was defined by subtracting a NDT from RTs.

④ As absolute coherence increased, choices were both more accurate and faster.

⑤ Choice biases across sessions from the DDM fits

monkey T: 14 [-0.7–27]% coherence

monkey A: -7 [-15–4]% coherence

R(Spearman’s correlation coefficient between values from logistic and DDM fits)=0.96, P = 2.7 × 10^(-56)

⑥  Slopes of the DDM psychometric function: 

monkey T: 0.6 [0.5–0.7]

monkey A: 0.7 [0.6-0.8]

R=0.70, P = 2.2 ×10^(-14)

→ these DDM fits implied relatively unbiased choices

3.2 Recording-site localization

1) From Fig. 3d

① ML frequency tuning increased at more posterior sites, whereas AL frequency tuning increased at more anterior sites.

② Neurons around the ML-AL border were tuned for low frequencies.

2) Our findings are robust to uncertainty about the specific location of the ML-AL border.

3.3 Neuronal stimulus sensitivity

1) From Fig. 4a: Coherence-dependent modulations at neuron level

Example ML neuron Example AL neuron
Preferred frequency high low
Coherence-dependent modulations

yes

coh.↑ → firing rate↑

yes

coh.↓ → firing rate↑

slightly later response onsets and more sustained

→ Both ML and AL auditory-driven responses were modulated by the frequency content of the stimulus.

2) From Fig. 4b: Coherence-dependent modulations at population level

→ Both ML and AL showing sensitivity to signed coherence that was most prominent just after stimulus onset for ML (steeper curves), but was more persistent throughout stimulus presentation for AL.

3)  From Fig.5a-b: Neurometric sensitivity to stimulus

① Receiver operating characteristic (ROC)-based ‘neurometric functions’

ROC value (%) : the probability that an ideal observer could use the spiking activity of an individual neuron to decide whether a given stimulus contained more high- or low-frequency tone bursts.

② Neurometric slopes (neurometric sensitivity): calculated from firing rates between stimulus onset and the inferred time of the decision commitment, median [IQR] 

ML AL Median difference
monkey T 0.3 [0.2–0.5] 0.3 [0.1–0.4] P = 0.46, no significant difference
monkey A 0.4 [0.3–0.6] 0.4 [0.2–0.5] P = 0.13, no significant difference

 From Fig. 5b, the neurometric slopes tended to increase from just after stimulus onset until around the time of decision commitment, then turned to decresease.

From the insets of Fig. 5b, The neurometric slopes were similar for the two brain regions and the two monkeys (similar distribution of black bars).

→ ML and AL had similar neurometric sensitivity to the frequency content of the stimulus.

⑤ Median psychometric slopes (psychometric sensitivity) from all sessions: 

both monkeys = 0.8 [0.7–1.0]

⑥ Neurometric slopes were slightly lower than the corresponding psychometric slopes for the two brain regions and the two monkeys (all P<0.001).

→ On average, single-neuron ML and AL spiking activity was sensitive to stimulus coherence (neurometric sensitivity), but less so than psychometric sensitivity.

⑦ Either ML or AL activity could be pooled to improve sensitivity and provide the evidence needed to make the decision.

4) From Fig. 5c-e: session-by-session correlation between neurometric sensitivity and psychometric sensitivity

① the profiles were roughly similar in ML and AL

In both cases, they peaked around the inferred time of the decision commitment.

② the effects were statistically more reliable in AL

Especially in Fig. 5c, when aligned to stimulus onset, significant regression coefficients (red lines) appeared in AL but not in ML.

→There is a slightly closer association for AL versus ML activity and perceptual performance.

③ ML and AL stimulus-driven responses are similar, and either or both could, in principle, be used to inform the monkeys’ decisions.

3.4 Neuronal choice sensitivity

1) Choice probabilities of individual neurons

Quantifies the ability of an ROC-based ideal observer to use spiking activity to discriminate between low- and high-frequency.

2) From Fig. 6a: Neurons modulated by choices

ML neurons AL neurons

Partial neurons

(black bars in Fig.6a)

20% (9 of 45)

P < 0.05, significant modulation

31% (17 of 55)

P < 0.05, significant modulation

Average level

P = 0.41

no significant modulation

P = 0.38

no significant modulation

① Certain ML and AL neurons were modulated by the monkeys’ choices.

② No systematic effect of choice on the spiking activity of populations of ML and AL neurons.

3) From Fig. 6b-e: Correlation between choice probability and neurometric sensitivity (slope) of individual neurons

① Significant regression coefficients appeared in AL but not in ML (red lines in Fig. 6b-d and Spearman's ρ≥0.50 with P<0.01 in Fig. 6e)

 The most sensitive AL neurons had task-driven activity that was positively related to the monkey’s choices.

② This positive correlation was evident during stimulus listening (Fig. 6b) around the inferred time of decision commitment (Fig. 6c) and persisted after the inferred time of movement initiation.

③ AL activity was more related to the monkeys’ decision-making behavior than ML activity.

3.5 Microstimulation

Preferred frequency high-freq. low-freq.
Expected direction left right
Joystick movement contralateral ipsilateral
comparing to ML and AL (both from right hemisphere)

1) From Fig. 7a: Modulation of microstimulation at single-site level

Preferred frequency: high-freq. of monkey T

Single-site examples in AL Single-site examples in ML

Shift

/choice

horizental translation of psychometric curve after stimulating

38% (P=0.001)

significant leftward shift

choices of high-freq.↑

2% (P>0.05)

no significant shift

∆Slope

/sensitivity

-o.11 (P>0.05)

no significant change of sensitivity

0.07 (P>0.05)

no significant change of sensitivity

Preferred frequency: low-freq. of monkey A

Single-site examples in AL Single-site examples in ML

Shift

/choice

horizental translation of psychometric curve after stimulating

13% (P=0.03)

significant rightward shift

choices of low-freq.↑

-7% (P>0.05)

no significant shift

∆Slope

/sensitivity

-0.19 (P>0.05)

no significant change of sensitivity

0.09 (P>0.05)

no significant change of sensitivity

Four example sites illustrate:

① Microstimulation in AL (rather than ML) had a systematic effect on the monkeys’ choices, which means that the joystick movements tended to toward the expected side associated with the frequency after microstimulation.

→ Neurons with appropriate frequency tuning in AL, but not ML, provide sensory evidence used to form the decision.

② No significant changes were observed in the slope (sensitivity) of AL and ML after microstimulation.

2) From Fig. 7b: Modulation of microstimulation at population level

Consistent with the result ① at single-site level.

3) From Fig. 7c: Psychometric shift (Change in choice)

Microstimulation in AL shifted choices in the expected direction, median shift is 5% (P=0.01), but not in ML, 1% (P=0.45) across monkeys.

4) From Fig. 7d: Change in slope (sensitivity)

Both ML and AL microstimulation did not affect psychometric sensitivity (P>0.05).

→ AL had a more direct and causal role than ML in the formation of the decisions


4. Discussion

1) AL and ML have similar stimulus-driven responses (results in 3.3 ), but different causal relationships with a perceptual decision (AL was more related to the monkeys’ decision-making behavior and microstimulation, results in 3.4 and 3.5).

2) We do not know how and where in the brain this decision is formed, but it is likely that downstream brain regions (for example, the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) might accumulate
this evidence to form the decision.

3) Possible reasons for our findings inconsistent with previous researches that neural activity in auditory cortex is not reliably modulated by choice: differences in ① task designs, ② required decisions, ③ analysis of choice-related activity.


5. Conclusions

1) The ventral auditory pathway is functionally and causally involved in forming auditory perceptual decisions.

2) A simple, feedforward scheme might involve a representation of the acoustic features of a stimulus in the core auditory cortex and ML, which gets converted into task-relevant sensory evidence in AL.

3) The sensory evidence is used to form the decision in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.

【Day35 文献精读】Causal contribution of primate auditory cortex to auditory perceptual decision-making相关推荐

  1. 文献精读丨GWAS+TRN多组学方法揭示小麦穗发育调控过程

    文献精读笔记 英文题目:Systematic mining and genetic characteriza:tion of regulatory factors for wheat spike de ...

  2. seer文献_文献精读|基于SEER数据库构建肝癌非转移Nomogram

    文献精读|基于SEER数据库构建肝癌非转移Nomogram 作者:白介素2 摘要 目的 文章的目的比较明确,寻找肝癌肺转移相关的风险因素,并建立预后 Nomogram. 方法 回顾性分析 SEER数据 ...

  3. 文献精读丨NP 小麦群体演化历史Population genomics unravels the Holocene history of bread wheat and its relatives

    文献精读丨NP 面包小麦及其近缘种全新世群体演化历史 本期分享2023年3月17日发表于Nature Plant上的一篇关于面包小麦及其近缘种全新世群体演化历史的文章,讲述了小麦起源的故事,笔记总计约 ...

  4. 文献精读(1)MRD,NSCLC

    文献题目:Longitudinal Undetectable Molecular Residual Disease Defines Potentially Cured Population in Lo ...

  5. 文献精读-PSEP-生物质和PE塑料的共热解MD以及DFT模拟

    Title: A study on co-pyrolysis mechanisms of biomass and polyethylene via ReaxFF molecular dynamic s ...

  6. 【Day34 文献精读】Of magnitudes and metaphors: Explaining cognitive interactions between space, time,and..

    阅读文献: Winter, B., Marghetis, T., & Matlock, T. (2015). Of magnitudes and metaphors: Explaining c ...

  7. 【Day24 文献精读】Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors

    阅读文献: Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: understanding time through spatial metaphors. c ...

  8. 【Day1/5 文献精读】Speed Constancy or Only Slowness: What Drives the Kappa Effect

    阅读文献: Chen Y, Zhang B, Kording KP (2016) Speed Constancy or Only Slowness: What Drives the Kappa Eff ...

  9. 【Day6-7 文献精读】A unifying Bayesian framework accounting for spatiotemporal interferences with a ...

    阅读文献: Chen, Y., et al. (2021). "A unifying Bayesian framework accounting for spatiotemporal int ...

最新文章

  1. 使用QQ截图取色的方法
  2. 每日一皮:妹子们写的代码,Bug为什么这么多?
  3. mqtt异步publish方法
  4. ios pan手势滑动消失动画_IOS UIPanGestureRecognizer手势使用及识别状态UIGestureRecognizerState...
  5. windows无法安装iis信息服务器,windows server 2016 安装iis教程
  6. 注册机是如何获取序列号的?
  7. Unity模拟行星轨道(一)
  8. android开源人脸识别插件,face-android-demo
  9. HandyJSON和SwiftyJSON的应用
  10. 电子发票专用的邮箱—理票侠
  11. 【HANA系列】SAP 一位SAP培训顾问的建议:SAP HANA应该如何学习?
  12. 《数字图像处理》第三章学习总结感悟1:灰度变换与空间滤波概念及常用灰度变换方法介绍
  13. Lync / Skype for Business登录界面UI切换
  14. 【弱监督显著目标检测论文】Weakly-Supervised Salient Object Detection via Scribble Annotations
  15. Ubuntu安装搜狗输入法后修改默认英文输入状态的方法
  16. JAVA获取字符串首字拼音和全拼
  17. NoSQL在腾讯海量数据中的应用与实践
  18. java字符串流与管道流
  19. getApplicationContext 详解
  20. 十个你可能不知道的CloudFlare免费CDN加速技巧

热门文章

  1. UE4 将项目压缩成最小的Zip压缩包
  2. uC/OS和uClinux的比较
  3. PostgreSQL数据库存储结构
  4. C++ typename详解
  5. 基于交替迭代法的通用型交直流潮流计算方法(matlab版+python版)
  6. 每天都在用电脑、玩手机的你,现在的眼睛有多干?
  7. python unpacking_Python函数调用时unpacking参数特性
  8. 计算机桌面图标怎么显示出来,显示桌面图标不见了怎么办?显示桌面图标不见了解决方法...
  9. openssl 交叉编译 armv7内核
  10. Java抽象类与接口的运用